There is a pervasive issue in the way that animal research is applied to humans, especially with schizophrenia and psychedelics. At least this is the area of research where I was exploring when I become bothered. I’m sure this problem exists quite ubiquitously throughout most domains of animal biopsychology. Though, translating the conscious experience of rats through their behaviors does seem a little bit more absurd than translating genetic research from animals to humans.

LSD-Rats

One of the studies I was looking at was exploring an chronic LSD induced model of psychosis (1). David E. Nichols and colleagues found that giving chronic doses of LSD to rats for 3 months ended up producing lasting changes to genes and behavior. These changes lasted far after the drug was cleared from the animals systems. In this study, the researchers noted that the rats were not favoring sucrose over water and they conclude that this may be evidence of anhedonia, a common symptom of schizophrenia. The LSD rats also had increased exploratory behavior and locomotor behavior.

“You can tell that the rats were depressed because they did not crave sugar.”

Anhedonic?

There is a problem here. Anhedonia is typically associated with laziness and depressive behavior, even in rat models (2). It is a symptom of lacking hedonic responses; the inability to feel pleasure. The symptom is often related to apathy, and especially not curiosity and exploration, which are motivated behaviors. The lack of sucrose affection found in the LSD rats may be related to the way LSD-type drugs seem to rapidly abolish addiction (3). Many addiction models have explored how addictive drugs increase the peptide dynorphin (4), which is associated to withdrawal-like symptoms such as anxiety (5), depression (6), stress (6), dysphoria (7), and even psychosis (8). LSD has been shown to attenuate the effects of dynorphin’s main target receptor system, the kappa opioid receptors (KORs) (9).

The way LSD suppresses dynorphin/KOR activity may occur through 5HT2ar-mGlur2 complexes in which 5HT2a receptor agonism leads to suppression of mGlu2 receptor activity (10), since mGlu2 receptors have been found to facilitate dynorphin activity (11). 5HT1a receptor agonism also seems to suppress the increase of dynorphin that usually accompanies L-Dopa administration (12), so this may be another way that LSD attenuates dynorphin activity.

The effects of KOR agonism are much more like anhedonia than what was observed in the rats of the LSD-psychosis study. Dynorphin has been described as an antireward before (13), so its function to induce anhedonia makes sense as the term literally means lack of hedonic response, lack of reward. It makes sense pharmacologically because dynorphin suppresses dopamine (14), opposes the functions of the rewarding opioid systems (15), and seems to induce depressive behavior.

It is possible that the lack of sucrose-preference could be described as ‘anhedonic’ although this may not be like the anhedonic states that those with schizophrenia experience, which may look more like laying in bed, not moving for hours on end. Instead, the rats seem driven to explore for far longer than the control rats, which does not seem schizophrenic. Those with schizophrenia and depression have decreased motor activity and exploration, meanwhile those with bipolar disorder have been characterized as having motor hyperactivity and increased exploratory behavior (41). So LSD may better model mania/bipolar disorder rather than schizophrenia, something I’ve argued in Psychedelics and Schizophrenia.

Sweet Cravings

Amphetamine is often explored as a psychosis model in animals (16). The psychostimulant is known to induce hyperactivity in rats (17), which may be due to dopaminergic effects. As with other rewarding and addictive drugs, amphetamine increases dynorphin activity, which seems to be due to stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors and glutamate NMDA receptors (18). Even sugar increases dynorphin secretion (19), which may be part of how sugar becomes addictive (20). Since LSD is found to attenuate KOR mediated depressive effects (9) and also rapidly attenuates addiction (3), it may be that the LSD rats in the LSD-psychosis study are not developing this addiction response due to lasting changes to the KOR system. The locomotor activity found in amphetamine rats may be sign of increased dopamine activity, meanwhile hallucinogenic effects produced by amphetamine may be a byproduct of increased dynorphin activity, since agonism of KOR is psychotomimetic and hallucinogenic (21).

Drug cravings seem to get induced by dynorphin activity (4), which makes sense as a euphoriant may be able to help relieve or cope with dysphoria and stress. It would make sense if the LSD-rats were not experiencing sensitivity to stress or depressiveness, and thus not reaching for the sugar to cope. Instead, maybe the animals are more motivated towards novelty, thus the exploratory behaviors. To clearly show the distinction of novel-reward seeking and comfort-reward seeking, consider that stress often makes rats express less exploratory behavior (23) and locotomor behavior (22), but it does enhance sugar intake (24). This makes sense, even among humans we see those who are stressed reaching for the sugary delights. Perhaps sugar intake actually measures depressiveness/stress and the rats in the study are experiencing “anhedonia” because they are simply too happy (sarcasm).

It is possible that serotonergic drugs, such as SSRIs and psychedelics, could induce a reduced reward-seeking state. SSRI drugs have been observed to reduce reward and aversion processing (42), meanwhile dynorphin/KOR agonism and stress both seem to be aversive and potentiate the rewarding effects of drugs, and they do so by inducing reuptake of serotonin (43), which is the opposite of effect of SSRI drugs. Keep in mind, KOR agonism and stress also induce behavioral depression (44). Also, it is important to consider that humans and animals may seek out or have enhanced relief from the dysphoria of dynorphin by taking euphoriants. Perhaps dysphoria is like a tolerance break from euphoria or even baseline neutral affect.

Without suffering, perhaps reward is less desirable.

Outdated Models

I’ve talked a lot in the past how dynorphin can bind together so many of the schizophrenia hypotheses and even is found to be elevated in the brains of schizophrenics (25, 26). The dynorphin system is also the main target of the hallucinogenic drug Salvia Divinorum (21). Dynorphin isn’t only psychomimetic through KOR agonism, but it also binds to NMDAr directly as an antagonist (27), much like PCP and ketamine, which are both used as models for psychosis. KOR agonism also potentiates dopamine D2 receptor activity (28) and mimics some of the effects of dopamine D2 autoreceptors, namely dopamine release inhibition (29, 30).

A new study has even challenged schizophrenic animal research (31). The authors noted that locomotor activity due to psychostimulants such as amphetamine is highly associated with increased limbic striatal dopamine activity, whereas the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, such as hallucinations, is associated with high dopamine in the associative striatum. Despite this, researchers continue to use locomotor activity as evidence of positive symptoms in animal research.

Amphetamines may increase psychotic-like mechanisms by enhancing dynorphin to potentiate D2 activity, NMDAr antagonism, downstream dopamine release suppression which may disrupt working memory and cognitive abilities by preventing D1-NMDAr complex function (33). This is also evidenced by the fact that dopamine increases and abnormalities exist prior to the onset of psychotic symptoms (31), suggesting that it takes time for psychotic symptoms to emerge from hyper-dopaminergic activity, possibly suggesting it is downstream of dopamine, perhaps dynorphin.

In contrast, Salvia Divinorum, a dynorphin agonist, induces radical alterations to perception and consciousness that occur acutely (21). The dynorphin blocking drug buprenorphine also function as a rapid-acting and potent anti-psychotic (34, 35). Though the withdrawals of this drug also produces psychotic effects (36, 37, 38), which suggests that targeting KORs with antagonists is only a temporary solution to schizophrenia that is prone to dependency. Most fascinatingly, there is one case report that reported LSD-like effects from sublingual buprenorphine administration (39), although the subject was addicted to heroin and so this complicates matters.

From that case report (39):

On the second day, patient reported an intense psychologic experience, which he described as an “LSD-like trip.” He described a heightened sense of excitement, intense euphoria, experiencing radiant colors, trance music, and visualizing fairies and goblins playing around him. This lasted for nearly 4 to 6 hours and left the patient feeling intensely happy. After this, buprenorphine was withheld and the patient reported no recurrences after that. After 4 days, when the buprenorphine was restarted, the patient again reported a similar “trip”; however, this time it was less intense.

This effect of buprenorphine may be a kind of ‘psychedelic manic’ state, as opposed to a ‘dysdelic psychotic’ state like the one Salvia produces. This is particularly fascinating because it fits with the ideas proposed in Psychedelics and Schizophrenia, which suggests that there is an inverse relationship between serotonergic and dynorphinergic tone in the brain and that psychedelia is actually mania.

We must consider research on psychedelics and schizophrenia much more carefully than we currently do. There is this frequent assumption that psychedelics induce a state that is like schizophrenia without consideration that there exists multiple altered states that involve broadly similar effects, at least at face value. In reality, Salvia is very distinguishable from psychedelics, even so much that animals are able to distinguish between psychedelics and Salvia (40).

One study (21) actually compared the subjective effect reports of Salvia and psychedelic users:

The inhalation of vaporized salvinorin-A led to very strong psychotropic effects of rapid onset and short duration. Perceptual modifications included the visual domain, and in contrast with 5HT2A agonists, auditory hallucinations were very common. Also in contrast with the classical serotonergic psychedelics, loss of contact with external reality was prominent with the participants being unreactive to external visual and verbal cues, especially after the medium and high doses. While at the low and medium doses there was an increase in bodily sensations, at 1.0mg there was an almost complete loss of body ownership and an increase in out-of-body experiences. These results suggest that the dynorphins – KOR system may play a previously underestimated role in the regulation of sensory perception, interoception, and the sense of body ownership in humans.

It may be that Salvia is a better model for schizophrenia than either dissociatives, psychedelics, or psychostimulants. I’ll refer you to my dynorphin hypothesis of schizophrenia if you are curious to see why I think psychedelics could actually help to treat schizophrenia.

. . .

If you found this useful, consider joining the Patreon!

Special thanks to the seven patrons: Sarah Gehrke, Melissa Bradley, Morgan Catha, Niklas Kokkola, Abhishaike Mahajan, Riley Fitzpatrick, and Charles Wright! Abhi is also the artist who created the cover image for Most Relevant. Please support him on instagram, he is an amazing artist! I’d also like to thank Annie Vu, Chris Byrd, and Kettner Griswold for your kindness and making these projects and the podcast possible through your donations.

If you’d like to support these projects like this, check out this page.

If you liked this, follow me on

Twitter

You can also follow the discussion for this post on Reddit:

r/DrugNerds

r/SlateStarCodex

r/MiniWorlds (The Cover Photo)

CITATIONS

1. Marona-Lewicka, D., Nichols, C. D., & Nichols, D. E. (2011). An animal model of schizophrenia based on chronic LSD administration: old idea, new resultsNeuropharmacology61(3), 503-512.

2. Rygula, R., Abumaria, N., Flügge, G., Fuchs, E., Rüther, E., & Havemann-Reinecke, U. (2005). Anhedonia and motivational deficits in rats: impact of chronic social stressBehavioural brain research162(1), 127-134.

3. Johnson, M. W., & Griffiths, R. R. (2017). Potential therapeutic effects of psilocybinNeurotherapeutics14(3), 734-740.

4. Chavkin, C., & Koob, G. F. (2016). Dynorphin, dysphoria, and dependence: the stress of addictionNeuropsychopharmacology41(1), 373.

5. Crowley, N. A., Bloodgood, D. W., Hardaway, J. A., Kendra, A. M., McCall, J. G., Al-Hasani, R., … & Lowell, B. B. (2016). Dynorphin controls the gain of an amygdalar anxiety circuitCell reports14(12), 2774-2783.

6. Knoll, A. T., & Carlezon Jr, W. A. (2010). Dynorphin, stress, and depressionBrain research1314, 56-73.

7. Land, B. B., Bruchas, M. R., Lemos, J. C., Xu, M., Melief, E. J., & Chavkin, C. (2008). The dysphoric component of stress is encoded by activation of the dynorphin κ-opioid systemJournal of Neuroscience28(2), 407-414.

8. Clark, S. D., & Abi-Dargham, A. (2019). The role of dynorphin and the kappa opioid receptor in the symptomatology of schizophrenia: A review of the evidenceBiological psychiatry86(7), 502-511.

9. Sakloth, F., Leggett, E., Moerke, M. J., Townsend, E. A., Banks, M. L., & Negus, S. S. (2019). Effects of acute and repeated treatment with serotonin 5-HT2A receptor agonist hallucinogens on intracranial self-stimulation in ratsExperimental and clinical psychopharmacology27(3), 215.

10. dos Santos, R. G. (2014). Psychedelics, Glutamate, and Neuroimaging StudiesThe Journal of the American Society of Anesthesiologists120(6), 1521-1522.

11. Liu, N. J., Murugaiyan, V., Storman, E. M., Schnell, S. A., Kumar, A., Wessendorf, M. W., & Gintzler, A. R. (2017). Plasticity of signaling by spinal estrogen receptor α, κ-opioid receptor, and metabotropic glutamate receptors over the rat reproductive cycle regulates spinal endomorphin 2 antinociception: relevance of endogenous-biased agonismJournal of Neuroscience37(46), 11181-11191.

12. Tomiyama, M., Kimura, T., Maeda, T., Kannari, K., Matsunaga, M., & Baba, M. (2005). A serotonin 5-HT1A receptor agonist prevents behavioral sensitization to L-DOPA in a rodent model of Parkinson’s diseaseNeuroscience research52(2), 185-194.

13. Koob, G. F. (2009). Dynamics of neuronal circuits in addiction: reward, antireward, and emotional memoryPharmacopsychiatry42(Suppl 1), S32.

14. Tejeda, H. A., & Bonci, A. (2019). Dynorphin/kappa-opioid receptor control of dopamine dynamics: Implications for negative affective states and psychiatric disordersBrain research1713, 91-101.

15. Pan, Z. Z. (1998). μ-Opposing actions of the κ-opioid receptorTrends in pharmacological Sciences19(3), 94-98.

16. Robinson, T. E., & Becker, J. B. (1986). Enduring changes in brain and behavior produced by chronic amphetamine administration: a review and evaluation of animal models of amphetamine psychosisBrain research reviews11(2), 157-198.

17. Geyer, M. A., Russo, P. V., Segal, D. S., & Kuczenski, R. (1987). Effects of apomorphine and amphetamine on patterns of locomotor and investigatory behavior in ratsPharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior28(3), 393-399.

18. Hanson, G. R., Singh, N., Merchant, K., Johnson, M., & Gibb, J. W. (1995). The role of NMDA receptor systems in neuropeptide responses to stimulants of abuseDrug and alcohol dependence37(2), 107-110.

19. Josefsen, K., Buschard, K., Sørensen, L. R., Wøllike, M., Ekman, R., & Birkenbach, M. (1998). Glucose stimulation of pancreatic β-cell lines induces expression and secretion of dynorphinEndocrinology139(10), 4329-4336.

20. Hoebel, B. G., Avena, N. M., Bocarsly, M. E., & Rada, P. (2009). A behavioral and circuit model based on sugar addiction in ratsJournal of addiction medicine3(1), 33.

21. Maqueda, A. E., Valle, M., Addy, P. H., Antonijoan, R. M., Puntes, M., Coimbra, J., … & Barker, S. (2015). Salvinorin-A induces intense dissociative effects, blocking external sensory perception and modulating interoception and sense of body ownership in humansInternational Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology18(12), pyv065.

22. Pinzón‐Parra, C., Vidal‐Jiménez, B., Camacho‐Abrego, I., Flores‐Gómez, A. A., Rodríguez‐Moreno, A., & Flores, G. (2019). Juvenile stress causes reduced locomotor behavior and dendritic spine density in the prefrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in Sprague–Dawley ratsSynapse73(1), e22066.

23. Berridge, C. W., & Dunn, A. J. (1989). Restraint-stress-induced changes in exploratory behavior appear to be mediated by norepinephrine-stimulated release of CRFJournal of Neuroscience9(10), 3513-3521.

24. Dess, N. K., & Choe, S. (1994). Stress selectively reduces sugar+ saccharin mixture intake but increases proportion of calories consumed as sugar by ratsPsychobiology22(1), 77-84.

25. Heikkilä, L., Rimón, R., & Ternius, L. (1990). Dynorphin A and substance P in the cerebrospinal fluid of schizophrenic patientsPsychiatry research34(3), 229-236.

26. Moustafa, S. R., Al-Rawi, K. F., Al-Dujaili, A. H., Supasitthumrong, T., Al-Hakeim, H. K., & Maes, M. (2020). The Endogenous Opioid System in Schizophrenia and Treatment Resistant Schizophrenia: Increased Plasma Endomorphin 2, and κ and μ Opioid Receptors are Associated with Interleukin-6.

27. Chen, L., Gu, Y., & Huang, L. Y. (1995). The opioid peptide dynorphin directly blocks NMDA receptor channels in the ratThe Journal of physiology482(3), 575-581.

28. Escobar, A. P., González, M. P., Meza, R. C., Noches, V., Henny, P., Gysling, K., … & Andrés, M. E. (2017). Mechanisms of kappa opioid receptor potentiation of dopamine D2 receptor function in quinpirole-induced locomotor sensitization in ratsInternational Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology20(8), 660-669.

29. Bruijnzeel, A. W. (2009). kappa-Opioid receptor signaling and brain reward functionBrain research reviews62(1), 127-146.

30. Tang, L., Todd, R. D., & O’Malley, K. L. (1994). Dopamine D2 and D3 receptors inhibit dopamine releaseJournal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics270(2), 475-479.

31. Kesby, J. P., Eyles, D. W., McGrath, J. J., & Scott, J. G. (2018). Dopamine, psychosis and schizophrenia: the widening gap between basic and clinical neuroscienceTranslational psychiatry8(1), 1-12.

32. Howes, O. D., Kambeitz, J., Kim, E., Stahl, D., Slifstein, M., Abi-Dargham, A., & Kapur, S. (2012). The nature of dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia and what this means for treatment: meta-analysis of imaging studiesArchives of general psychiatry69(8), 776-786.

33. Nai, Q., Li, S., Wang, S. H., Liu, J., Lee, F. J., Frankland, P. W., & Liu, F. (2010). Uncoupling the D1-N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor complex promotes NMDA-dependent long-term potentiation and working memoryBiological psychiatry67(3), 246-254.

34. Schmauss, C., Yassouridis, A., & Emrich, H. M. (1987). Antipsychotic effect of buprenorphine in schizophreniaThe American journal of psychiatry.

35. Maremmani, I., Pacini, M., & Pani, P. P. (2006). Effectiveness of buprenorphine in double diagnosed patients. Buprenorphine as psychothropic drugHeroin Addiction & Related Clinical Problems8(1), 31-48.

36. Karila, L., Berlin, I., Benyamina, A., & Reynaud, M. (2008). Psychotic symptoms following buprenorphine withdrawalAmerican Journal of Psychiatry165(3), 400-401.

37. Weibel, S., Mallaret, M., Bennouna-Greene, M., & Bertschy, G. (2012). A case of acute psychosis after buprenorphine withdrawal: abrupt versus progressive discontinuation could make a differenceJ clin psychiatry73(6), e756.

38. Navkhare, P., Kalra, G., & Saddichha, S. (2017). Possible psychosis associated with buprenorphine withdrawalJournal of Clinical Psychopharmacology37(6), 748-749.

39. Saddichha, S., Subodh, B. N., & Chand, P. K. (2016). Sublingual buprenorphine-induced psychomimetic effectsAmerican journal of therapeutics23(1), e242-e243.

40. Killinger, B. A., Peet, M. M., & Baker, L. E. (2010). Salvinorin A fails to substitute for the discriminative stimulus effects of LSD or ketamine in Sprague–Dawley ratsPharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior96(3), 260-265.

41. Henry, B. L., Minassian, A., Young, J. W., Paulus, M. P., Geyer, M. A., & Perry, W. (2010). Cross-species assessments of motor and exploratory behavior related to bipolar disorderNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews34(8), 1296-1306.

42. McCabe, C., Mishor, Z., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2010). Diminished neural processing of aversive and rewarding stimuli during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatmentBiological psychiatry67(5), 439-445.

43. Schindler, A. G., Messinger, D. I., Smith, J. S., Shankar, H., Gustin, R. M., Schattauer, S. S., … & Chavkin, C. (2012). Stress produces aversion and potentiates cocaine reward by releasing endogenous dynorphins in the ventral striatum to locally stimulate serotonin reuptakeJournal of Neuroscience32(49), 17582-17596.

44. Zhang, Y., Butelman, E. R., Schlussman, S. D., Ho, A., & Kreek, M. J. (2005). Effects of the plant-derived hallucinogen salvinorin A on basal dopamine levels in the caudate putamen and in a conditioned place aversion assay in mice: agonist actions at kappa opioid receptorsPsychopharmacology179(3), 551-558.

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out /  Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out /  Change )

Connecting to %s